1 Introduction

The assimilation of immigrants into the culture of their host country represents one of the most pertinent issues facing national policy makers. In the European Union (EU), immigration represents an important phenomenon. According to the latest Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) International Migration Outlook (OECD 2013), increased migration flows in 2011–2012 mainly consisted of people moving within the EU, and Germany is the top destination. Although these waves of immigration may be temporary phenomena stemming from the global economic downturn, the effects on the social and economic status of the host countries might be persistent.Footnote 1

When people migrate to a new country, they bring with them resources, habits, and experiences from their home country, including valuable productive inputs (e.g., labor, skills, other sources of human capital) that can generate substantial economic returns for the host country.Footnote 2 Yet, they also enter this new society with (potentially) different cultural backgrounds, marked by distinct languages, religions, and (economic and social) values. Unfortunately, cultural exchanges triggered by migration flows sometimes are associated with dissimilation and segregation, which exert detrimental (social and economic) effects on both natives and immigrants.Footnote 3 In an analysis of data from the National Identity Survey during 1995–2003, Bianchi et al. (2012) determine that the majority of natives in OECD countries express worries about the impact of migration inflows on their social security; the proportion of respondents noting concerns ranged from as low as 40 % in the UK to as high as 80 % in Norway (see also Martinez and Lee 2000; Bauer et al. 2000).Footnote 4 According to the 2008 European Social Survey (OECD, 2010) almost 50 % of Germans believe that immigrants have not had positive effects on the national economy. A recent poll comparing immigration perceptions across the UK and Germany confirmed that about 20 % of Germans feel that immigration is the most pressing issue facing the country, compared with 40 % in the UK.Footnote 5

Psychology and sociology research, in turn, analyzes the negative effects of perceived discrimination and cultural dissimilation on the social stress and mental health of immigrants (Thomas and Hughes 1986; Vega and Rumbaut 1991; Rumbaut 1994; Finch et al. 2000; Taylor and Turner 2002; Sellers et al. 2003; Hughes and Demo 1989). Social conflict between natives and specific communities of immigrants in major European countries highlight the potential damage that can result from neglecting the effects of cultural dissimilation, including unsustainable social conditions and substantial economic losses.Footnote 6

Despite extensive analyses of the effects of migration flows on equilibrium conditions for the labor market of the host country (Harris and Todaro 1970; Borjas 1994; Ortega 2000), only recently have social scientists started to recognize the economic value of harmonious cultural diversity (see Ottaviano and Peri 2006; Bisin et al. 2011) and the importance of cultural, linguistic, and ethnic homogeneity in determining people’s openness to immigrants (Card et al. 2012).

We therefore take an immigrant perspective to investigate the association between assimilation with the host culture and subjective well-being, drawing data from ten waves of the German Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP), a longitudinal data set that provides information about both economic (income, labor status) and non-economic (demographic, education, health) conditions. It also asks immigrants to self-report (on a 5-point scale) their assimilation with the culture of their host country, that is, their perceived closeness with a German and with their foreign identity, as well as their proficiency with German and their native language. Thus, we depart from an empirical tradition that uses indirect measures, such as the wage gap between natives and immigrants or time since migration (see Chiswick 1978; Carliner 1980; Pischke 1992; Licht and Steiner 1994; Schmidt 1993, 1997; Bauer et al. 2005; Constant and Massey 2005; Fertig and Schurer 2007; Gundel and Peters 2007), to rely on a more direct, subjective measure of perceived assimilation.Footnote 7

Using self-reported measures of well-being and cultural assimilation raises some additional methodological issues, mainly related to the effects of unobserved individual heterogeneity on econometric estimates. Therefore, we rely on data gathered over multiple years so that we can control for differences in reporting styles across respondents and unobserved time-invariant personality traits. Nor is our study the first to use subjective measures of assimilation as determinants of immigrants’ living conditions. Also using the SOEP data set, Casey and Dustmann (2010) study the relationship between labor market outcomes (income and labor force participation) and the extent to which immigrants identify with the culture of the host country rather than their native country. Their findings indicate the strong transmission of identity across generations of immigrants but weak (non-significant) effects of cultural assimilation on economic outcomes. Combining information on language proficiency, ethnic self-identification, and other individual domains contained in the SOEP data set, Constant and Zimmermann (2008, 2009a) also define a subjective measure of the intensity of immigrants’ ethnic identity, the ethnosizer. With it, they classify migrants into four states: assimilation (strong identification with the host culture, weak identification with the culture of origin), integration (strong identification with both host and origin cultures), marginalization (strong detachment from both host and origin cultures), or separation (weak identification with the host culture, strong identification with the culture of origin). This status largely depends on pre-migration characteristics and is exogenous to immigrants’ work intensity. Furthermore, young migrants tend to be better assimilated or integrated than older migrants; Muslims and Christians do not integrate but assimilate best; and immigrants who attended college in their home country separate less. Other studies (Constant et al. 2006, 2009; Zimmerman 2007; Constant and Zimmerman 2008) affirm positive, significant impacts of assimilation with the host culture on economic behavior (e.g., work probability, earnings, home ownership).Footnote 8

Although motivated by the same interest in the effects of assimilation, we depart from previous studies by proposing that labor outcomes provide only a partial description of the conditions of immigrants. As economic literature suggests (see Frey and Stutzer 2002a, b; van Praag and Ferrer-i-Carbonell 2004; Bruni and Porta 2005; Dolan et al. 2008), money is not enough to make people happy; in addition to economic factors, non-economic dimensions play a crucial role in determining subjective well-being. Accordingly, we uncover the positive, strong association of cultural assimilation with immigrants’ subjective well-being. Identifying with one’s own culture of origin exerts only marginally significant effects for specific categories of respondents, but the self-reported level of life satisfaction is positively and significantly associated with the extent to which immigrants identify with the German culture and can communicate in German language.

This main result is robust to several specifications that account for potential confounding factors. First, we control for multiple individual dimensions, including the respondent’s demographic, educational, social, economic, and health conditions. The positive association between assimilation with the host country and life satisfaction remains strong, even after controlling for economic dimensions (employment status and income) considered by prior literature. Our findings thus suggest that, beyond labor market conditions, a significant linkage between the life satisfaction of immigrants and their perceived cultural assimilation remains unexplored. Second, we investigate the interplay of cultural assimilation with the time since immigration (Gundel and Peters 2007; Safi 2010; Herrero et al. 2011), to assess whether the association between cultural assimilation and immigrants’ subjective well-being differs among “recent” immigrants, who are more likely to perceive differences between their native and German cultures; “established” immigrants who have been exposed to a long cultural assimilation process since their arrival; and second-generation immigrants. Assimilation with the German culture emerges as significantly associated with the level of life satisfaction only for established and second-generation immigrants; for recent immigrants, we instead detect a negative, significant relationship between their subjective well-being and the extent to which they identify with their native culture. This finding confirms the apparent difficulties immigrants experience when exposed to the (different) culture of the host country. Third, we check whether our result is mediated by the external social conditions of immigrants in Germany or concerns about foreigners’ situation. For the former, we include a set of regional controls that capture immigrants’ contributions to the society where they live, namely, the percentage of immigrants in the total population in the region and the percentage of (un)employed immigrants on the total (un)employed population in the region. For the latter, we use proxies for perceived discrimination and the lack of economic aspirations of immigrants. The estimates of these variables are significant and exhibit the expected signs, but their inclusion in the main specification does not alter the magnitude of the relationship between German identity and life satisfaction.

The rest of this article is organized as follows: Section 2 introduces the data, a simple theoretical framework we use to derive testable hypotheses, and the empirical strategy. We present the estimation results in Section 3, then conclude in Section 4 with some final considerations.

2 Cultural assimilation and subjective well-being: testable predictions, data, and empirical strategy

2.1 A simple theoretical framework

With our simple theoretical framework, we seek to better highlight the link between immigrants’ assimilation into the host country culture and their subjective well-being. Our framework is based on a model proposed by Stark and Jakubek (2013) to study the interplay of integration, relative (material) deprivation, and immigrants’ optimal investment in human capital. Rather than conducting a formal equilibrium analysis, we focus on the main assumptions of the original model and adapt them to our context to derive meaningful, testable hypotheses.

Consider a population of homogeneous immigrants who live in the host country, and let F and N denote the sets of immigrants and native agents in this country, respectively. Conducting a profitable economic and social life (i.e., finding a job, accessing fundamental public services, engaging in civic life) requires the acquisition of destination-specific human capital. Let x denote the level of human capital that an immigrant can acquire; we assume x ∈ [0,1 − 𝜃 x 0], where x = 1 is the human capital of a native, and 𝜃 x 0, where 𝜃, x 0 ∈ [0,1], represents the fraction of the stock of human capital carried by the immigrant from the country of origin that can be converted into destination-specific human capital. Acquiring destination-specific human capital is costly, so c C(x) denotes the cost of assimilating, with c > 0 and C(x) denoting a convex function. For simplicity, we convert destination-specific human capital (x + 𝜃 x 0) into earnings on a one-to-one basis. In turn, we denote the subjective well-being of an immigrant with the following (separable) function of the investmentin destination-specific human capital:

$$ \text{SWB}(x)=Y(x+\theta x_{0})-\gamma \text{CD}(x+\theta x_{0})-cC(x)+\text{OF} $$
(1)

The first term on the right denotes a strictly concave function that captures the well-being the immigrant obtains from income. The OF term captures other (constant) immigrant-specific factors that positively affect subjective well-being. It is the second term on the right that represents the distinctive element of the model: CD(x + 𝜃 x 0) is cultural dissimilation with respect to the host culture, as experienced by the immigrant, and γ ≥ 0 is immigrants’ sensitivity to CD (x + 𝜃 x 0). Even in this case, we assume that destination-specific human capital gets converted into cultural capital on a one-to-one basis, to simplify the model. Coherent with the original formulation proposed by Stark and Jakubek (2013), we assume that cultural dissimilation increases with the difference between the average destination-specific human capital of each immigrant’s reference group and the immigrant’s own level: \(\text {CD}(x~+~\theta x_{0})~=~\sum \limits _{j\in J}\max (0;x_{j}~-~x~-~\theta x_{0})\), where x j is the average human capital of the reference group j and J is the set of the reference groups. Stark and Jakubek (2013) use definitions of reference groups to distinguish non-integrated and integrated immigrants: The former includes in a reference group only fellow immigrants living in the same host country, such that J = {F}. If populations were homogeneous, the immigrant would not experience any cultural dissimilation with respect to other members of the community: CD(x + 𝜃 x 0) = 0. For the latter group of integrated immigrants, both fellow immigrants and the native population represent reference groups, such that J = {N, F}. If we assume homogeneous populations and normalization of natives’ human capital, immigrants only experience cultural dissimilation with respect to the native population: CD(x + 𝜃 x 0) = max(0; 1 − x𝜃 x 0).

With their original formulation, Stark and Jakubek (2013) study optimal investments in destination-specific human capital, chosen by the immigrant to maximize subjective well-being. Non-integrated immigrants, whose reference group only includes fellow immigrants, make relatively limited investments in human capital because they do not suffer any deprivation (or cultural dissimilation, in our framework) relative to natives. However, integrated immigrants, who include natives in their reference group, invest more in destination-specific human capital to reduce their material (or cultural, in our framework) gap with natives.

Although highly stylized, this theoretical framework provides meaningful, testable predictions about the association between assimilation with the host culture and the subjective well-being of immigrants. With our empirical exercise, we thus aim to assess whether cultural assimilation is positively associated with the level of subjective well-being per se, after we control for several potential confounding factors, such as the immigrant’s (subjective) characteristics and other socio-economic conditions. Additional qualifications also apply when we account for the integration status of immigrants. As suggested by Eq. 1, we expect the association between assimilation with the host culture and subjective well-being to be stronger for integrated than for non-integrated immigrants.

2.2 Data and empirical strategy

To study the relationship between cultural assimilation and immigrants’ subjective well-being, we used data from the German Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP), a longitudinal survey that has been collecting information about the socio-economic status, health, and well-being of private households since 1984 (see Wagner et al. 2007). An interesting aspect of this survey is that it oversamples the resident migrant population in Germany, offering a unique source of information about the living conditions of immigrants over time. These data feature heterogeneity with respect to the country of origin of immigrants; the top five origins are Turkey, ex-Yugoslavia, Greece, Italy, and Spain.

To capture subjective well-being (SWB), the questionnaire asks, “How satisfied are you with your life, all things considered?” and the responses range from 0, indicating “completely dissatisfied,” to 10, or “completely satisfied.” Flourishing literature investigates the determinants of subjective well-being using self-reported evaluations of life satisfaction or happiness, and one of the most intriguing issues pertains to how the conclusions change when the focal question refers to happiness (a proxy of emotional well-being) rather than the more general concept of life satisfaction. As Kahneman and Deaton (2010, p. 16489) note, “emotional well-being (sometimes called hedonic well-being or experienced happiness) refers to the emotional quality of an individual’s everyday experience—the frequency and intensity of experiences of joy, fascination, anxiety, sadness, anger, and affection that make one’s life pleasant or unpleasant. Life evaluation refers to a person’s thoughts about his or her life.”Footnote 9 The relevance of the longitudinal dimension of the SOEP data set—which stretches over 30 years for some respondents—prompted us to focus on the more general, retrospectively oriented (rather than emotionally variable) concept of subjective well-being, measured as the self-reported level of satisfaction with life as a whole. It must be pointed out that subjective well-being measures are not exempt from potential criticisms mainly related to their validity (the degree to which these measures succeed in capturing the conditions of the respondent), reliability (the degree to which survey techniques secure consistent data upon repeated application), and comparability over time and over respondent samples (see Sachs, 2013, for a discussion of these issues). Our paper does not contribute to this methodological discussion and we rely on the established literature which validates the use and interpretation of these measures. Rather, by following Kahneman and Krueger (2006) and Layard (2005), we believe that subjective well-being measures present, in our context, two main advantages relative to standard objective indicators. First, in a policy perspective, they allow for more direct welfare analysis that can be conducted on specific social groups, such as immigrants living in the host country. Second, these measures lead to a shift in emphasis from the importance of standard economic variables (such as income and job status) to more subjective and socially oriented determinants of individuals’ well-being, such as social contacts and civic participation, cultural orientation and health conditions.

Next, to measure cultural assimilation, we relied on direct information about immigrants’ sense of identification with the host culture and their German language proficiency. Following Casey and Dustmann (2010), we built a measure of cultural assimilation using information from a question that asked immigrants to report how strongly “German” they felt, on a scale from 1 (“not at all”) to 5 (“completely”). The gauge of writing and speaking skills also used a 5-point scale, from 1 (“no knowledge at all”) to 5 (“very good knowledge”). We measured language proficiency as the average of the writing and speaking scores. We also included SOEP information about immigrants’ identification with the native culture and proficiency in the native language, to control for the potential interplay between host and native cultures.

Figures 1 and 2 show the Epanechnikov kernel density of life satisfaction, according to the strength of the identification with Germany and the native country, respectively. A strong score corresponds to the categories “mostly” and “completely,” whereas the weak score refers to all other categories. The probability mass is more concentrated at high satisfaction levels for those with strong identification with the host country than for those with a weak German identity. Yet, the contrary is true for foreign identity: The densities in Fig. 2 look similar, but that for people with a weak home identity is more concentrated at higher levels of life satisfaction.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Epanechnikov kernel density estimate of life satisfaction by sense of German identity (SOEP 2003). A strong score corresponds to the categories “mostly” and “completely,” whereas the weak score refers to all other categories

Fig. 2
figure 2

Epanechnikov kernel density estimate of life satisfaction by sense of native identity (SOEP 2003). A strong score corresponds to the categories “mostly” and “completely,” whereas the weak score refers to all other categories

As we show in Fig. 3, the previously documented life satisfaction gap between natives and immigrants (Baltatescu 2007; Amit 2010; Bartram 2010; Safi 2010) depends on the level of cultural assimilation of immigrants and reverses for those who feel completely integrated in German society. A Kolmogorov-Smirnov test also shows that the differences in the distributions in Figs. 1, 2, and 3 are all statistically significant at the 1 % level.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Epanechnikov kernel density estimate of life satisfaction—Germans and immigrants by sense of identity (SOEP 2003)

These figures seem to confirm a positive relationship between cultural assimilation and immigrants’ subjective well-being at a descriptive level, yet we need a more formal analysis to account for potential confounding factors. Therefore, in our empirical analysis, we focus on first- and second-generation immigrants and estimate a linear panel data model with individual and time fixed effects:

$$ \text{SWB}_{i,t}=\alpha_{i}+\alpha_{t}+w_{it}^{\prime}\beta+\text{assimilation\_host}_{it}^{\prime}\gamma+\varepsilon_{i,t} $$
(2)
$$\bigtriangledown i=1,..., n\text{ \ \ and }\bigtriangledown t=1,..., T $$

where i = 1,...,n indicates individual respondents; t = 1,...,T indicates the survey year; and w i t is a vector of control variables. In line with the theoretical framework we presented in the previous subsection, we focus on the sign and significance of the γ parameters and test whether assimilation with the host culture is positively and significantly associated with immigrants’ subjective well-being, γ > 0.

With individual and time fixed effects (FE), we control for time-invariant, unobserved, individual heterogeneity and time-related common shocks. The first component, individual FE, is crucial for addressing potential differences in reporting styles across respondents (Angelini et al. 2014). Thus, a relevant methodological issue is that measures of life satisfaction might not be interpersonally comparable if different people interpret and use distinct response categories for the same subjective question, a phenomenon known as differential item functioning (Holland and Wainer 1993). As long as the reporting style used by each individual respondent does not vary over time, individual fixed effects can account for this bias. Individual FE also provides a control for every unobserved factor that refers broadly to the “character” of the person, does not vary over time, and is likely to affect both self-reported life satisfaction and subjective measures of cultural assimilation, such as personal traits, optimism, religion, and ethnicity.Footnote 10 The German identity variable shows substantial within-respondent variation over time: 64.55 % of the immigrants in our sample show at least a one-unit change in the German identity variable during the survey period. By employing a linear specification, we also treat life satisfaction as a cardinal rather than ordinal construct, such that we can carry out the fixed effects analysis using the “within” estimator for the whole sample, and the results are easier to interpret (see Clark et al. 2008). From a practical perspective, assuming cardinality or ordinality of life satisfaction has no significant effect on the results (Ferrer-i-Carbonell and Frijters 2004).Footnote 11

The individual characteristics that we control for when estimating the association between life satisfaction and cultural assimilation include the previously detailed, subjective measures of identification with the culture of origin, demographics, health, and socio-economic status. The demographic variables include a quadratic polynomial for age,Footnote 12 the number of own children, a dummy that takes a value of 1 if the respondent is married, a dummy identifying divorced people, and another dummy defining whether a person has been widowed. We measure the person’s objective health status as the number of annual doctor visits and a dummy that indicates whether the person spent at least one night in the hospital in the previous year. Finally, the set of variables aimed to define individual socio-economic status include income and dummies for whether the person has a full-time job, a part-time job, or is undergoing a program of vocational training (a residual category includes people who are not working at all).

Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics for all the variables we used in the regressions. The immigrants in our sample are mostly married (72.2 %) and have one child on average; 58.3 % work either full- or part-time, 3.7 % are in vocational training, and the remaining 38 % are outside the labor force. The average immigrant is 38.9 years of age and went to the doctor three to four times in the previous year. Our final sample contains an unbalanced panel of 22,636 observations (6167 persons) from 10 survey waves over 18 years (1985, 1986, 1987, then every 2 years until 2003, except 1993).Footnote 13

Table 1 Descriptive statistics

In the second part of our analysis, in line with our theoretical framework, we tested whether the positive association between assimilation with the host culture and subjective well-being is stronger for integrated than non-integrated immigrants. We use the time since migration as a proxy for social integration and repeat the estimation procedure on three subsamples of immigrants: recent immigrants who have been in Germany for less than 10 years and are likely not to be fully integrated, established immigrants who migrated to Germany more than 10 years ago and have been exposed to a sufficiently long integration process since their arrival,Footnote 14 and second-generation immigrants, who have only an indirect migration background.

Finally, we checked whether controlling for additional external social conditions affects the magnitude or sign of the association between cultural assimilation and life satisfaction. For this effort, we used data collected by Eurostat for the European Labour Force Survey and aggregated at the regional level.Footnote 15 To measure general openness in a region to foreign-born members, we constructed indexes by region and year for the concentration of immigrants, defined as the percentage in the total population (Fig. 4). We also split this general index into the concentration of (un)employed immigrants, defined as the percentage of (un)employed immigrants on the total un(employed) population. If immigrants contribute actively to the host society through the labor force, society should be more open to them, such that non-natives should feel more accepted overall. These data are available only since 1995, and when we introduce them in the analysis, our sample drops to 11,262 observations (4444 persons). On average, immigrants represent about 10 % of the total population and 21 % (9 %) of the unemployed (employed) population in Germany.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Total immigrant concentration in German regions (European Labour Force Survey 1995–2003 average)

The regions hosting more immigrants relative to their population are concentrated in south-west Germany—the richest area of the country. Probably due to their historical heritage, characterized by closed attitudes toward external influences, and the renewed strength of anti-immigration political parties, the northeast regions instead display a very low concentration of immigrants. More recent years of the SOEP also provide information about the extent to which immigrants are concerned about the situation for foreigners in Germany. On average, 32.8 % of immigrants are very concerned, though this variable shows a downward trend over time, with the exception of a temporary increase in 2001, probably due to the 9/11 attacks.

3 Results

We present our main estimation results in Table 2. In the first column, we start by estimating a life satisfaction equation in which we control for standard determinants of subjective well-being. Our findings are consistent with prior literature, confirming the importance of economic conditions for determining immigrants’ well-being, in that people with higher income and in full-time employment are better off on average in terms of life satisfaction. However, non-economic factors also play a crucial role. In particular, poor health conditions relate strongly and negatively to life satisfaction. In terms of demographics, we find a U-shaped relationship between age and life satisfaction, with a minimum age around 52 years, as in Blanchflower and Oswald (2008). Being married is positively associated with life satisfaction; the number of children does not seem to affect the well-being of immigrants though.

Table 2 Lifesatisfaction of immigrants (SOEP data 1985–2003)—estimation with individual and time FE

In the next columns, we shift to our main analysis focus: the relationship between assimilation with the host culture and the subjective well-being of immigrants. For this assessment, we introduce measures of ethnic identity (German and foreign identity) and language proficiency. The results support our main theoretical prediction, because after we control for the traditional determinants of life satisfaction and individual and time fixed effects, stronger (perceived) assimilation with the host culture is associated with higher levels of life satisfaction. The sense of belonging to the German culture is very important, but identifying with the native culture does not have significant effects on life satisfaction.Footnote 16 This result is substantively meaningful; it implies that moving from not feeling at all German to feeling completely German has 1.2 times the impact on life satisfaction as does being married (column 3). Being able to interact with local citizens also is very important, such that German language proficiency has a strong, significant, positive effect on immigrants’ well-being, even when we control for socio-economic conditions and labor market status. Importantly, as we control for time-invariant personality traits and response styles by including individual fixed effects, our results are not driven by spurious correlations of cultural assimilation and life satisfaction.Footnote 17

These results also can inform existing literature about immigrants’ enclaves and social integration. Danzer and Yaman (2013) show that by limiting immigrants’ civic participation and interactions with native populations, ethnic enclaves in Germany significantly reduce their cultural assimilation. Dustmann (1996) documents that immigrants from more distant cultural backgrounds (e.g., Turks, Greeks) exhibit a lower assimilation tendency into German society. Language proficiency and education are other important determinants of cultural assimilation, as is labor market integration (Dustmann, 1994, 1996). In light of this literature, our results highlight the importance of discouraging the creation of ethnic enclaves but supporting host country language proficiency. Such actions can foster immigrants’ cultural assimilation per se and also benefit their indirect association for their well-being.

In Table 3, we present the results of a repeated analysis with the three subsamples, defined according to their migration background, in which we sought to address the interplay of assimilation with the host culture and time since migration (Gundel and Peters 2007; Safi 2010; Herrero et al. 2011). In line with our second testable prediction, assimilation with German culture is significantly associated with the level of life satisfaction of established and second-generation immigrants only. Among recent immigrants, we instead detect a negative and significant relationship between subjective well-being and the extent to which they identify with their native culture, confirming the difficulties experienced by immigrants upon their exposure to the (different) culture of the host country. Cultural assimilation matters especially in the long run though, and effective integration policies might accelerate this process.

Table 3 Life satisfaction of immigrants (SOEP data 1985–2003) by migration background and years since migration—estimation with individual and time FE

Finally, as we show in Table 4, we checked the robustness of our results after introducing controls for the social conditions in which immigrants live. In particular, spatial effects driven by regional differences in openness to foreign-born populations might determine differences in immigrants’ life satisfaction, rather than a direct link between subjective perceptions of cultural assimilation and their subjective well-being. To understand the relative importance of these two mechanisms, we augment our prior specification to include the concentration of immigrants in each German region. In addition, the general openness of the region to foreign-born residents could affect their life satisfaction, but the potential contributions that immigrants make to the local society also might affect the interplay between cultural assimilation and their life satisfaction. Therefore, we split the index according to two variables that measure the concentration of unemployed and employed immigrants. These variables came from the European Labour Force Survey and vary by region (according to the NUTS1 European territorial classification) and year. Unfortunately, data from the European Labour Force Survey are only available since 1995, so this second part of the analysis is restricted to a relatively recent subsample of the SOEP. When focusing on these recent years, we also can control for perceived discrimination, measured with a dummy variable equal to 1 if the immigrant is “very concerned for the foreigners’ situation in Germany” and 0 otherwise.Footnote 18

Table 4 Life satisfaction of immigrants (SOEP data 1995–2003)—estimation with individual and time FE

We repeat the same estimation as in the last column of Table 2 with the new sample, to show that our findings are qualitatively the same even when we focus on a different time period. We introduce an index of immigrant concentration by region and year, measured as the percentage of immigrants in the total population of the region. With this variable, we aim to capture the extent to which the relation between cultural assimilation and life satisfaction is affected by the region’s general openness to foreign-born persons. As we noted, we want to disentangle the interplay of the direct impact of subjective perceptions of cultural assimilation on immigrants’ happiness from the indirect effect of a regional-level, general openness toward foreign-born persons.

Our first empirical observation reveals that the concentration of immigrants has no statistically significant impact on life satisfaction, but the effects of German identity and language proficiency remain unaltered. Cultural assimilation is an internal mechanism, based on perceptions and not indirectly influenced by external conditions. A further, more detailed analysis adds that when we distinguish the level of concentration of immigrants by employment status, external social conditions start to matter for the well-being of immigrants, insofar as they represent the potential contributions of immigrants to the society in which they live. If immigrants in the region contribute to the labor force, it positively affects their happiness, but greater unemployment is detrimental to immigrants’ well-being. We measured these two effects with the concentration of employed immigrants and the concentration of unemployed immigrants, respectively. Individual-level variables capturing cultural assimilation also affirm their importance in explaining the well-being of immigrants, and these results remain robust to the inclusion of a proxy for perceived discrimination, which shows the expected negative sign

3.1 Robustness checks

We conducted several sensitivity analyses to check the robustness of our results. All the results discussed in this section are available on request. First, we reestimated our model, using a fixed effect probit-adapted ordinary least squares (FE-POLS) approach, as developed by van Praag and Ferrer-i-Carbonell (2008). This approach circumvents violations of the cardinality assumption and is computationally easier than an ordered probit. It uses a “rough cardinalization” of the ordered life satisfaction variable and calculates the relative cumulative frequencies of life satisfaction and the corresponding z scores. For each original variable, the value of the roughly cardinalized life satisfaction variable comes from taking the conditional expectation of a standard, normally distributed, continuous variable, given the original ordering (for an example, see Cornelißen 2009). The results using this FE-POLS approach, reported in Table 5, are qualitatively the same as those obtained with an FE-OLS approach.

Table 5 Life satisfaction of immigrants (SOEP 1985–2003)—estimation using the probit-adapted OLS method (POLS) with individual and time FE

Second, in Table 6 we checked if the results might be driven by particular groups of immigrants. Immigrants coming from other German-speaking countries might be very different from other immigrants, because their culture is more similar to that of the host country, and they face no language barrier. Therefore, we reestimated our regression from the third column of Table 2 but excluded immigrants from Austria, Luxembourg, or Switzerland and those born in Germany. The coefficients for the German identity and German proficiency variables decreased slightly (0.080 and 0.158, respectively) but the results stayed qualitatively unchanged. We also tried excluding or only including immigrants from the top five origin countries (Turkey, ex-Yugoslavia, Greece, Italy, and Spain). In both cases, we confirmed our results; if anything, the effect of cultural assimilation with the host culture grew even more important when we excluded immigrants who came from these top five countries of origin. A variable that assigned, to each individual and year, the share of immigrants from the same country in that year (computed from the SOEP data) exerted a negative effect on life satisfaction (at the 10 % significance level), but the effects of the other variables remained the same.

Table 6 Life satisfaction of immigrants (SOEP 1985–2003)—estimation with individual and time FE

Third, because we include many years of data, panel mortality might lead to a selectivity bias. For example, immigrants who are not well integrated in Germany or are not satisfied with their life might decide to return to their country of origin and drop out of our sample. Therefore, we checked whether attrition bias is a problem, by applying Verbeek and Nijman’s 1992 methodology, which consists of adding selectivity dummies to the main equation of interest. The selectivity dummy in year t for individual i equals 1 if that person participates to the survey in year t and also in year t + 1, while it is equal to 0 if the person drops out of the sample. With the null hypothesis of no selective attrition, selectivity dummies should be jointly insignificant, which we can test with a simple F test. In our case, we fail to reject the null hypothesis (for the specification in column 3 of Table 2, the p value for the selectivity dummies is 0.4342); therefore, attrition bias is not an issue. Our results are in line with those of Abolhassani and Alessie (2013), who also do not find evidence of attrition when using the SOEP data to study life satisfaction around retirement. As a further check, we considered whether the immigrant was planning to return to the country of origin by adding a dummy variable, equal to 1 if the immigrant planned to stay in Germany forever and 0 otherwise (last column of Table 6). This variable was significantly associated with life satisfaction, but the other results remained qualitatively unchanged. The effect of German identity fell somewhat (coefficient = 0.065), probably due to the relatively high correlation between the feeling of identification with the German culture and the wish to stay in Germany forever.

Fourth, we used other proxies for the openness of the region to immigration and general external conditions. A potential criticism of immigrant concentration variables in previous analyses is that they might be interpreted as enclave measures: New immigrants tend to settle in areas where there are other immigrants. In our case, this issue is less of a concern, because the regions we study are quite large. However, we used the share of extreme right-wing votes in parliamentary elections at the state level as an alternative measure of regional openness to immigration. Following Knabe et al. (2013), we obtained data from the German Federal Returning Office and constructed a new variable that combined the share of all votes for four extreme right-wing parties: the Republikaner (REP), the Deutsche Volksunion (DVU), the Nationaldemokratische Partei Deutschlands (NPD), and the Partei Rechtsstaatlicher Offensive (Schill-Partei). State-level elections occur every 4 years, so we had data about 1994, 1998, and 2002. We assigned the 1994 election results, by state, to the 1993 and 1995 waves of SOEP; those from 1998 to the 1997 and 1999 waves; and the results from 2002 to the 2001 and 2003 waves. Table 7 shows that the coefficient of the share of extreme right-wing votes appeared insignificant, and our other results remained unchanged, possibly due to the low variability of this measure in our sample. We also tried to add the regional gross domestic product (GDP) per capita at current prices to the regression,Footnote 19 but its effect was insignificant. We already had included time dummies in the regressions, and the time trend of the GDP per capita did not differ much across regions in the era we study.

Table 7 Life satisfaction of immigrants (1995–2003)—estimation with individual and time FE. Robustness of the results to the inclusion of the percentage of extreme right-wing votes and the (log) GDP as measures for external conditions

4 Conclusion

Immigration is an increasingly important phenomenon for European societies and also across the Atlantic, especially if it fuels risks of social instability. A recent OECD (2013) report on international migration suggests that the issue of cultural dissimilation determines how well a society can tackle the phenomenon. From this perspective, our analysis adds an important contribution to current academic and policy discussions, by highlighting the link between a self-reported but direct measure of cultural assimilation and the subjective well-being of immigrants.Footnote 20

Our contribution goes beyond previous socio-economic research, with two main novel results. First, we indicate a direct association between cultural assimilation and immigrants’ subjective well-being, unmediated by labor market outcomes (e.g., employment status, wages), time-invariant unobserved individual characteristics, or regional controls that capture the external social conditions of immigrants. Second, we show that the potential benefits of cultural assimilation go beyond the time dimension of the integration process, as widely debated in sociological literature, because its association with life satisfaction is stronger for established immigrants than for recent ones.

The analysis thus has some important policy implications. Successful immigration policies must account for the fundamental issue of cultural assimilation. Proper interventions to facilitate cultural assimilation (e.g., language courses, interventions to limit ethnic enclaves, civic and cultural activities to foster interactions between migrants and natives, educational programs) may have great potential in terms of increasing immigrants’ well-being. This issue is particularly urgent in Germany, where in recent years, “the political class had begun to accept that Germany is an immigration country with a responsibility to integrate immigrants fully into national life.”Footnote 21